Tag Archives: Critical Thinking

CRITICAL THINKING — THE GREAT MENTAL MODELS Part One

One trait setting humans apart from other species on this planet is thought. Next to our closest competitors, the octopus and the orangutan, humans far surpass at deep cognitive processing and complex problem solving. Recently, there’s a significant breakthrough in aiding human advancement to understand and know general reality concepts. It’s a four-volume tutorial called Critical Thinking — The Great Mental Models.

Shane Parrish is an internet thought leader and author of The Great Mental Models. Shane also hosts Farnam Street and The Knowledge Project Podcast. His sites’ taglines are Master the Best of What Other People Have Already Figured Out and The Best Way to Make Intelligent Decisions.

Shane opens the Mental Models series with, “Education doesn’t prepare you for the real world.” He says, “The key to better understanding the world is to build a latticework of mental models.” Mental models, according to Shane Parrish, describe the way the world works in simplicity. They fundamentally, and without complication, shape how we think, how we understand, and how we form beliefs.

Largely subconscious, mental models operate beneath the surface. We’re not generally aware of them, and yet when we look at a problem, they’re the reason we consider some factors relevant and others irrelevant. They are how we infer causality, match patterns, and draw analogies. They are how we think and reason.

A mental model is a compression of how something works. Any idea, belief, or concept can be distilled down. Like maps, mental models reveal key information while ignoring the nonessential.

I’ve subscribed to Farnam Street and The Knowledge Project Podcast for the last six years. The insight and value I’ve received, particularly in creativity, has been far greater than the price of admission. So, it was with great anticipation that I waited for last Christmas when Shane Parrish and Penguin released the four-volume Great Mental Models in hardcover.

For the last three months, I’ve been immersed in the critical thinking concepts loaded throughout the Mental Models pages. And over the years (sixty-eight of them), I’ve developed a research and learning style that (for me) truly pays. I’m able to accurately absorb and retain information, converting facts into useful knowledge, through a simple process that I’d like to share in a two-part post.

Part One is an overview of Shane Parrish’s Mental Models and how my system applies. Part Two, being published two weeks from now on 19Apr2025, will be a much deeper dive into how to work the Great Mental Models and how they can work for you.

Two profound influences on Shane Parrish are Charlie Munger and Peter Kaufman. Munger, who recently passed away at age 99, was the co-founder of Berkshire Hathaway with the famed investor Warren Buffet. Charlie Munger was among the finest thinkers of the past century, and he died a multi-billionaire.

Peter Kaufman is also a wealthy man. Kaufman is an entrepreneur, venture capitalist, and acclaimed writer. Along with Parrish, he edited many of Charlie Munger’s latticework of mental model essays and assembled them into a book titled Poor Charlie’s Almanac. It’s from these essays that the Knowledge Project took life. Now it’s evolved into The Great Mental Models.

Throughout Mental Models, you’ll see a constant reference to “Latticework”. It’s worth an initial definition:

The latticework concept essentially revolves around building a toolkit of frameworks from various disciplines to help improve decision-making and problem-solving abilities.

The idea is that no single mental model is sufficient to tackle all the complex problems the world presents. Instead, having a broad and interconnected set of models from diverse fields—such as economics, physics, biology, psychology, and more—allows one to view problems from multiple perspectives. This multidisciplinary approach can significantly enhance one’s ability to understand, adapt, and react effectively to different challenges.

Charlie Munger popularized this approach in his famous speech “The Psychology of Human Misjudgment,” where he discusses the importance of these models in helping to avoid cognitive biases and make better decisions. By developing a latticework of mental models, one can better connect the dots between seemingly unrelated information and insights, leading to more effective and informed decision-making.

Simply stated, The Great Mental Models synthesize timeless wisdom from various disciplines to empower readers/thinkers to make better decisions, solve complex problems, and think more clearly. Clarity of thought is a goal even the greatest minds strive for. Although many people use many methods, they all come down to a fundamental reality—the information they seek is available if they apply the foundation of critical thinking.

Before we get into the overall outline of what’s in The Great Mental Models, I want to share two indisputable truths that shaped my worldview and exponentially boosted my thought input and output processes. This change in critical thinking attracted opportunities I never would have found without properly understanding and working with, not against, these two opposing forces. One is compounding. The other is entropy.

Compounding and entropy are fully covered in The Great Mental Models. Cleverly, Shane Parrish sprinkled bits and pieces of entropy and compounding details throughout the series. Although he deals with each principle on their own, he’s used compounding and entropy concepts/principles (not laws) as subtext. They are the true takeaways—supported by all the tools in the lattice framework—and he’s left it for you, the reader, to experience your “Ah-Ha!” moment when you understand the constructive and destructive forces of compounding and entropy that govern the universe.

Let me introduce you to a learning and retention system I’ve developed over the years that I use for all topics and projects. I’m not saying it’s perfect by any stretch or that it will change your game. I just (at this age) know myself, and I know what works for me because I’ve been using the system for a long time, and it has a proven ROI—return on investment.

For a project like digesting, understanding, and retaining timeless truths and wisdom found in Mental Models, I shelled out a hundred and twenty bucks US for the printed, hardcovered books—not the ebooks—and at the time of purchase (Christmas 2024) the paperbacks weren’t available. Check Amazon and you might find them on sale for less.

I learned a reading and retention trick from my mother who was an English teacher and avid book devourer. That’s to mark up the pages with underlines and notes. Being a visual learner, this method has the best returns (for me), and I use a process of red underlines, yellow highlights, and black hand-printed notes. Yes, I mark the hell out of the pages.

I also keep a notebook specific to the project where I’ll transpose content as I understand it along with my developing thoughts, questions, and “get-its”. I use an 80-sheet Mead-Cambridge 8 ½ x 11 spiral bound book with wide rules as it’s easy to handle and can be laid flat without the thing wanting to spring itself closed. See pictures.

Besides notes, I also photocopy diagrams, pictures, and passages from the hardcovers then cut and paste them onto the notebook pages. I’m sure that to a stranger sifting through my stuff, they might think I’m doing a ketamine experiment but, hey, it works for me and I’m stickin’ to it.

I also search terms and ideas on the internet as I progress. If I find something useful, I’ll screenshot it and print n’ paste as well. And I’m not afraid to say I constantly use ChatGPT4.5 to clarify matters and bounce ideas off. Did I use Chat to write some of this post? Maybe. Maybe not. 😊

Something extremely useful in the Great Mental Models is the “Conclusion” at the end of each idea. It sums up the concept in clear and comprehensible language. I photocopy each conclusion and then tape it into the notebook. Again, being visual, it makes the material jump right out.

That’s the simple learning and retaining process I use. You’re more than welcome to own it. Now, let’s dive into the overall of what you can find in Mental Models that’s as rich in practical content as it is in intellectual treasure. Here’s what each volume brings to your table:

Volume 1: General Thinking Concepts

The opening act of this series, “General Thinking Concepts,” lays down foundational blocks for building a robust mental toolkit. It’s all about broad principles that apply across various fields. Think of it as the Swiss Army knife in your cognitive toolbox. Key models covered include:

  • Introduction: Acquiring Wisdom
  • The Map is Not the Territory
  • Circle of Competence
  • First Principles Thinking
  • Second-Order Thinking
  • Probabilistic Thinking
  • Inversion—Always Invert
  • Occam’s Razor
  • Hanlon’s Razor

This volume is a powerhouse that prepares your mind to think critically and avoid common, cognitive bias errors like misrepresentations or overly narrow focusing.

Volume 2: Physics, Chemistry, and Biology

In “Physics, Chemistry, and Biology” the series shifts from general thinking to the specific laws governing our physical world. This volume uses fundamental scientific principles to illuminate ideas that affect our everyday decisions and observations, including:

  • Relativity
  • Reciprocity
  • Thermodynamics
  • Inertia
  • Friction and Viscosity
  • Velocity
  • Leverage
  • Activation Energy
  • Catalysts
  • Alloying
  • Evolution One: Natural Selection and Extinction
  • Evolution Two: Adaptation Rate and the Red Queen Effect
  • Ecosystems
  • Niches
  • Self-Preservation
  • Replication
  • Cooperation
  • Hierarchical Organization
  • Incentives
  • Tendency to Minimize Energy Output

By exploring these foundation models, readers gain insights into how universal principles of physics, chemistry, and biology can guide better decision-making in both personal and professional spheres.

Volume 3: Systems and Mathematics

The third volume, “Systems and Mathematics”, brings a holistic view, emphasizing core ideas like the whole is greater than the sum of its parts. This volume is about understanding complex, mathematical systems and navigating them effectively such as:

  • Feedback Loops
  • Equilibrium
  • Bottlenecks
  • Scale
  • Margin of Safety
  • Churn
  • Algorithms
  • Critical Mass
  • Emergence
  • Irreducibility
  • Law of Diminishing Returns
  • Compounding
  • Sampling
  • Randomness
  • Regression to the Mean
  • Multiplying by Zero
  • Equivalence
  • Surface Area
  • Global and Local Maxima

A quote from Mental Models says it well. “In spite of what you majored in, or what the textbooks say, or what you think you’re an expert in, follow a system wherever it leads. It will be sure to lead across traditional disciplinary lines.”

Volume 4: Economics and Art

Finally in Volume 4, “Economics and Art”, we move from the theoretical to the practical. This is solid, real-life information that just may change your perspective on many things in life like:

  • Scarcity
  • Supply and Demand
  • Optimization
  • Trade-offs
  • Specialization
  • Interdependence
  • Efficiency
  • Debt
  • Monopoly and Competition
  • Creative Destruction
  • Gresham’s Law
  • Bubbles
  • Audience
  • Genre
  • Contrast
  • Framing
  • Rhythm
  • Melody
  • Representation
  • Plot
  • Character
  • Setting
  • Performance

This volume, with fascinating insights on economics and art (fiction writing included), wraps up the series by empowering readers to see beyond individual elements and appreciate the interconnectedness of all things—physical and metaphysical.

Each of The Great Mental Models books builds on the previous, crafting a comprehensive guide designed to sharpen your analytical abilities and decision-making skills. By the end, you’re not just equipped with theoretical knowledge, but with practical tools applying across your personal life, professional practice, and beyond.

Shane Parrish put a tremendous effort into producing this latticework frame. Reading and absorbing Mental Models might transform how you critically think about the world—and how you interact with it. It certainly did for me, and the three months of immersion in Mental Models were the most cerebral exercises in my life.

Watch for Part Two of Critical Thinking — The Great Mental Models on Saturday 19Apr2025 at 8:00 am PST on DyingWords.net where we’ll do a deep-end dive into each of the fundamental concepts. It’s gonna be a long one.

WHAT MAKES CRAZY CONSPIRACY THEORISTS TICK?

A conspiracy theory is the belief that a plot by powerful people or an organization is working to accomplish a sinister goal—the truth of its existence secretly held from the public. Conspiracy theorists see authorities—governments, corporations, and wealthy people—as fundamentally deceptive and corrupt. Their distrust of official narratives runs so deep that they connect dots of random events into what they believe make meaningful patterns, despite overwhelming conflicting evidence, or absence of supporting evidence, to their conclusions. Aside from a lack of reason and common sense, what makes crazy conspiracy theorists tick?

First, let’s look at what Time Magazine identified as the prominent conspiracy theories believed by the American public. These were identified in a recent poll, and I’m not kidding you. Some people actually swallow these kooky concoctions.

From lowest to highest percentage, they are:

10. The Reptile Elite — Among us are flesh-eating, blood-drinking, shapeshifting, extraterrestrial reptilian humanoids bent on enslaving the human race. The British Royals and the Bush family are part of the group, as was Margaret Thatcher.

9. The CIA and AIDS — Thinking is that the Central Intelligence Agency was out to destroy homosexuals and blacks, so they invented the deadly HIV virus and injected it in place of hepatitis vaccinations.

8. Holocaust Revisionism — Most deniers accept that the internment camps existed but claim the numbers of people murdered are greatly exaggerated. General Eisenhower saw this coming when he forced hundreds of civilian witnesses to tour the camps and bear the truth to the world.

7. Jesus and Mary Magdalene — These folks claim Jesus and Mary Magdalene were a married couple, but Jesus occasionally shared her with his disciple, Peter. They attribute this to the Gnostic Gospels which were discovered in 1945 and claim it’s being covered up by the Vatican. Mainstream scholars dismiss as the Gnostic Gospels as even being authentic.

6. The Moon Landings Were Faked — According to this crowd, none of the Apollo missions happened. They were filmed on a Hollywood lot, or possibly at area 51. Watch this priceless clip of Buzz Aldrin punching conspiracy theorist Bart Sibrel in the face. Click Here

5. Secret Societies Rule the World — If you’re a member of the global elite, then you’d already know this. And you might belong to one or more of many groups; the Illuminati, Freemasons, Skull & Bones, Opus Dei, Bilderberg Group, or maybe even have a seat on the Council of Foreign Relationship. Sorry, your Costco card won’t cut it.

4. Paul McCartney is Dead — Supposedly the Beatles covered-up the real Sir Paul’s death in 1966 and an imposter has been in his place ever since. Maybe I’m Amazed, because I saw a recent clip of Paul McCartney in front of tens of thousands at Hyde Park takin’ a swing at Pretty Woman with Bruce Springsteen. If he’s an imposter, he’s some good at it.

3. Area 51 and Aliens — There’s a real Air Force base at Groom Lake, 150 miles north of Las Vegas, where all sorts of black op aircraft are tested. Like most military installations, public access is restricted, but you can get a good look at it on Google Earth. The resolution is excellent, but I couldn’t find any saucer-shaped craft or ET-looking creatures. Oh, right… they keep them inside… or maybe back at Roswell.

2. 9/11 Cover-Up — Apparently 42% of Americans believe the attacks on the New York, Washington, and Pennsylvania were orchestrated by some arm of the U.S. Government. Sad.

1. The JFK Assassination — The mother of conspiracy theories.  Times’ poll reports that only 32% believe that Lee Harvey Oswald acted alone. So, two-thirds of Americans truly think their 35th President’s murder is unsolved? How can that be?

Well, it comes down to mindset.

I’m not saying that conspiracies don’t happen—even at a mass scale. The Holocaust was a massive conspiracy to exterminate the Jews. The Nazis totally believed the Jews were a threat to their existence. It was an article of faith within the Third Reich.

9/11 was a monstrous conspiracy—orchestrated by Bin Laden and al-Qaeda. The Russian Revolution was a conspiracy. So was the American Revolution—fifty-six men signed the Declaration of Independence. Nixon conspired to hide Watergate. Abraham Lincoln was murdered through a conspiracy. So was Julius Caesar. Don’t forget Stalin, the Mexican Drug Cartels, and Scientology. And don’t get me going on Klaus Schwab with his Fourth Industrial Revolution, Davos, and the World Economic Forum.

If some conspiracies are true, then how do you determine which ones are false? The more these characteristics apply, the more likely the theory is wrong:

  • “Proof” of the conspiracy emerges through dot-connecting without any hard, physical evidence.
  • Execution requires large and complex elements.
  • The agents require nearly super-human powers.
  • Everyone maintains secrecy.
  • There is a grandiose ambition for control.
  • The plot ratchets from small to large events.
  • Everything has a sinister overtone.
  • Facts and speculation are mingled without assigning degrees of probability.
  • The theorist is extremely suspicious of authority—government and private.
  • The theorist refuses to consider alternative explanations, seeking only confirmation of the theory.

I understand the mindset of real conspirators. It’s all about money, power, and/or self-preservation. But what about the tin-foil hat crowd?

In American Conspiracy Theories, political scientists Joseph Uscinski and Joseph Parent conducted an “extensive empirical study” on the subject and found: Conspiracy Theorists are often caricatured as a small demographic composed primarily of middle-aged white male internet enthusiasts who live in their mother’s basements—but that’s wrong. Conspiracy theories permeate all parts of society and cut across age, gender, race, income, political affiliation, educational level, and occupational status.

What gives? How does a cross-section of should-be normal people get so distorted in their thoughts and believe in really weird things?

Quassim Cassam, who published a peer-reviewed paper title Conspiracy Theories, is a professor of philosophy at the University of Warwick. He’s embarking on a study on why people believe in outlandish theories. Part of this work is to answer why people get pushed to extremes like joining ISIS.

Professor Cassam says: It seems to be because of the kind of thinker they are, or to put it bluntly, because there’s something wrong with how they think. It’s the peculiarities of their intellectual constitution—in a word, their intellectual character. It’s what social psychologists call a conspiracy mentality.

Cassam goes on: The gullible rarely believe they’re gullible and the closed-minded don’t believe they’re closed-minded. Closed-mindedness is the toughest intellectual vice to tackle because it’s in its very nature to be concealed from those who have it. There’s no reasoning with those kinds of people.

I found an article in Sage Journals where Willem van Prooijen of the University of Amsterdam summed it: Conspiracy theorists tend to have one thing in common—they feel a lack of control over their lives.

Some DyingWords followers know I’m a life-long student of the John F Kennedy Assassination, and I’m completely satisfied beyond all doubt that Lee Harvey Oswald did it—acting alone. Part of my research was checking online chat boards on the JFK forums. Lemme tell you—there are some whacko, nut-jobs out there. One of them made repeated death threats to me via email for calling out BS in the JFK CT department.

I believe there are four reasons why people believe JFK’s murder was a conspiracy. These reasons probably apply to most conspiracy theories and theorists.

1. They don’t have the correct information to understand the case facts.

2. They haven’t got the personal knowledge, or experience, to properly interpret the evidence.

3. They simply want to believe in a conspiracy.

4, They don’t have the mental capacity for critical thought process.

Professor Cassam is right. There’s no reasoning with those kinds of people.

USING OCCAM’S RAZOR WITHOUT CUTTING YOUR OWN THROAT

Occam’s razor is the law of parsimony. It’s a mental model of reasoning. At its core, Occam’s—also spelled Ockham’s or Ocham’s—states, “Among competing hypotheses, the one with the fewest assumptions should be selected.” Simpler yet, when solving a problem, the simplest solution is usually the right solution… but not too simple lest you cut your own throat.

A razor is a philosophical principle, or heuristic (shortcut) rule of thumb, that allows you to cut to the chase of the matter and shave off, or eliminate, unlikely explanations for a phenomenon and/or avoid unnecessary actions. Razors include three reasoning forms: deductive, inductive, and abductive. We’ll get to those in a bit, but cutting to the chase of this piece, the simplest example of Occam’s razor is the duck test.

If it looks like a duck, walks like a duck, swims like a duck, has feathers, and quacks, then it’s probably a duck. There’s also the zebra test commonly used in medical fields to avoid unnecessarily complicated diagnosis—when hearing hoofbeats, think horses, not zebras.

That’s all fine and dandy when examining webbed birds and hooved mammals. How about using Occam’s razor in everyday situations like homicide investigations?

I was taught back in Murder School 101 to always use Occam’s razor for solving crimes. My mentor was an old sergeant named Fred Mahle. Wonderful guy, rest his soul. BTW, Fred was the RCMP detective who got child serial killer Clifford Olson to confess and turn over ten bodies for a fee of a hundred grand. Best business deal ever. Ten families got closure. Olson got life and painfully died in jail from pancreatic cancer.

Fred was right when he said, “Looking at murder cases, the simplest answer is usually the right answer, but be careful with that logic. It could backfire, and you could cut your own throat.” Fred also taught me that the more bizarre a situation, the closer the answer was to home. Additionally, Fred said, “You get more bees with honey than you do with vinegar” and, “You slide further on bullshit than you do on gravel”.

Anyway, back to Occam’s razor and how this logic works.

William of Occam was a fourteenth-century Franciscan friar who had trouble with conventional Christian doctrine like miracles. As a scholastic philosopher and theologian, William took a heuristic (mental shortcut) approach to explain the unexplainable. History credits William of Occam with wise quips like, “It is vain to do more with what can be done with fewer”, “A plurality is not to be posited without necessity”, “Circumstances being equal, that is better and more valuable which requires fewer”, and “Entities are not to be unnecessarily multiplied”.

Greats like Aristotle, Newton, Einstein, Hawking, and Sagan all bought into the law of parsimony—parsimony being defined as, “Quality of being careful with money or resources: Thrift. State of being stingy, economy in the use of a means to an end. Economy of explanation in conformity with Occam’s razor”. These thinkers knew the simplest explanation to a problem was preferable to one that’s more complex. (Usually).

Minds like these deferred to core mental models which are the way we see the world and interpret reality. Mental models include concepts like core competence, first principles thinking, thought experiment, second-order thinking, probabilistic thinking, inversion, and Occam’s razor. Great minds also employed the three forms of reasoning.

Deductive, inductive, and abductive reasoning support any analytical application of Occam’s razor. Rather than paraphrase what deductive, inductive, and abductive reasoning mean, I’ll simply copy and paste this piece from Merriam-Webster:

Deductive Reasoning

Deduction is generally defined as “the deriving of a conclusion by reasoning.” Its specific meaning in logic is “inference in which the conclusion about particulars follows necessarily from general or universal premises.” Simply put, deduction—or the process of deducing—is the formation of a conclusion based on generally accepted statements or facts. It occurs when you are planning out trips, for instance. Say you have a 10 o’clock appointment with the dentist and you know that it takes 30 minutes to drive from your house to the dentist’s. From those two facts, you deduce that you will have to leave your house at 9:30, at the latest, to be at the dentist’s on time.

Deductive reasoning always follows necessarily from general or universal premises. If a sandwich is defined as “two or more slices of bread or a split roll having a filling in between,” and a hot dog is defined as “a frankfurter; especially a frankfurter heated and served in a long split roll” then one must deduce that any hot dog served in a split roll is a sandwich.

Inductive Reasoning

Whereas in deduction the truth of the conclusion is guaranteed by the truth of the statements or facts considered (the hot dog is served in a split roll and a split roll with a filling in the middle is a sandwich), induction is a method of reasoning involving an element of probability. In logic, induction refers specifically to “inference of a generalized conclusion from particular instances.” In other words, it means forming a generalization based on what is known or observed.

For example, at lunch you observe 4 of your 6 coworkers ordering the same sandwich. From your observation, you then induce that the sandwich is probably good—and you decide to try it yourself. Induction is at play here since your reasoning is based on an observation of a small group, as opposed to universal premises.

Abductive Reasoning

The third method of reasoning, abduction, is defined as “a syllogism in which the major premise is evident but the minor premise and therefore the conclusion only probable.” Basically, it involves forming a conclusion from the information that is known. A familiar example of abduction is a detective’s identification of a criminal by piecing together evidence at a crime scene. In an everyday scenario, you may be puzzled by a half-eaten sandwich on the kitchen counter. Abduction will lead you to the best explanation. Your reasoning might be that your teenage son made the sandwich and then saw that he was late for work. In a rush, he put the sandwich on the counter and left.

If you have trouble differentiating deductioninduction, and abduction, thinking about their roots might help. All three words are based on Latin ducere, meaning “to lead.” The prefix de- means “from,” and deduction derives from generally accepted statements or facts. The prefix in- means “to” or “toward,” and induction leads you to a generalization. The prefix ab- means “away,” and you take away the best explanation in abduction.

Whether you reason by deduction, induction, or abduction, you can always learn from what the greats had to say about using Occam’s razor. Here are three reasonable quotes worth noting:

Albert Einstein referred to Occam’s razor when developing his theory of special relativity. He formulated his own version: “It can scarcely be denied that the supreme goal of all theory is to make the irreducible basic elements as simple and as few as possible without having to surrender the adequate representation of a single datum of experience. Or, everything should be made as simple as possible, but not simpler.”

Physicist Stephen Hawking advocated for Occam’s razor in his book, A Brief History of Time: “We could still imagine that there is a set of laws that determines events completely for some supernatural being, who could observe the present state of the universe without disturbing it. However, such models of the universe are not of much interest to us mortals. It seems better to employ the principle known as Occam’s razor and cut out all the features of the theory that cannot be observed.”

Isaac Newton used Occam’s razor, too, when developing his theories. Newton stated: “We are to admit no more causes of natural things than such as are both true and sufficient to explain their appearances.” Newton sought to make his theories, including the three laws of motion, as simple as possible, with only the necessary minimum of underlying assumptions.

Furthermore, simple is as simple does. A conclusion can’t rely just on its simplicity. It must be backed by empirical evidence. And when using Occam’s razor to make deductions, we must avoid falling prey to confirmation bias. In the case of the NASA moon landing conspiracy theory, for example, some people consider it simpler for the moon landing to have been faked, others for it to have been real.

Lisa Randall best expressed the issues with the narrow application of Occam’s razor in her book, Dark Matter and the Dinosaurs: The Astounding Interconnectedness of the Universe: “Another concern about Occam’s Razor is just a matter of fact. The world is more complicated than any of us would have been likely to conceive. Some particles and properties don’t seem necessary to any physical processes that matter—at least according to what we’ve deduced so far. Yet they exist. Sometimes the simplest model just isn’t the correct one. This is why it’s important to remember that opting for simpler explanations still requires work. They may be easier to falsify, but still require effort. And that the simpler explanation, although having a higher chance of being correct, is not always true.”

Occam’s razor is not intended to be a substitute for critical thinking. It is merely a tool to help make that thinking more efficient.

Harlan Coben has disputed many criticisms of Occam’s razor by stating that people fail to understand its exact purpose: “Most people oversimplify Occam’s razor to mean the simplest answer is usually correct. But the real meaning, what the Franciscan friar William of Ockham really wanted to emphasize, is that you shouldn’t complicate, that you shouldn’t “stack” a theory if a simpler explanation was at the ready. Pare it down. Prune the excess.”

Occam didn’t have the only razor in the think-tank shop. Other greats contributed to the critical thinking crowd. Here are some examples:

Hanlon’s razor — Never attribute to malice which can be adequately explained by stupidity.

Hitchen’s razor — What can be asserted without evidence can be dismissed without evidence.

Hume’s razor — What ought to be cannot be deduced from what is.

Alder’s razor — If something cannot be settled by experiment or observation, then it’s not worthy of debate.

Sagan’s razor — Extraordinary claims require extraordinary evidence.

Okay. This is all philosophical and eggheady. What about some real-life examples of critical thinking used to disprove flawed logic using Occam’s razor? To combat the Walmart of Weird Beliefs and the Grab Bag of Fake News? I’m not going to try and explain bizarre behavior like QAnon, Proud Boys, or crazy conspiracy theorists who wear tinfoil hats. As my wise, redneck brother-in-law Darryl says, “Ya can’t fix stupid.”

But I will touch on two high-profile puzzles that can be solved through the application of the law of parsimony—Occam’s razor. One is the JFK assassination. I’m a life-long student of that event and, at one time, I wasted hours debating the case on an internet board. Like Darryl says, “Ya can’t fix stupid”, so I gave up.

The Kennedy assassination brings out craziness in people. Conspiracy Theorists (CTs) will have you believe in a massive cover-up by the Illuminati, a hit by the Mafia, a plot by the military-industrial alliance, LBJ did it, and on and on and on and on…

Using Occam’s razor applied to the case facts, Lee Harvey Oswald acted alone. A deranged man brought his cheap rifle to work and shot the President of the United States—arguably the most powerful and important person in the world—from a tall building. Extraordinary claims require extraordinary evidence, right? After 57 years there hasn’t been one single irrefutable piece of evidence that anyone other than Oswald was involved. Nothing. The simplest answer here is the correct answer.

Another Occam’s application is explaining the enigma of Donald Trump. Trump supporters will have you believe the man is an impeccably-crafted genius with sight farther than the furthest horizon. Others, using Occam’s razor, simply say the man is a natural-born grifter and a common asshole. I know which explanation I’ll accept.

Remember, Occam’s razor is complemented by other mental models, including fundamental error distribution, especially Hanlon’s razor, confirmation bias, availability heuristic thinking, and hindsight bias. The nature of mental models is that they tend to all interlock in a lattice and work best in conjunction.

It is important to note, like any mental model, Occam’s razor is not foolproof. Like Fred said, use it with care, lest you cut your own throat. This is especially crucial when it comes to important or risky decisions. There are exceptions to any rule, and we should never blindly follow the results of applying a mental model which logic, experience, or empirical evidence contradict.

Using Occam’s razor, though, you’ll generally call out quackery with the duck test, and when you hear hoofbeats behind you, in most cases you should think horses, not zebras—unless you’re on the African savannah.